Associations between two words are usually the same no matter which word is the prompt

Balota, D. A., Yap, M. J., Cortese, M. J., Hutchison, K. A., Kessler, B., Loftis, B., Neely, J. H., Nelson, D. L., Simpson, G. B., & Treiman, R. (2007). The English lexicon project. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 445459.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Benson, M., Benson, E., & Ilson, R. (1997). The BBI dictionary of English word combinations. John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2018). Praat: Doing phonetics by computer (6.0.37) [Computer software]. http://www.praat.org/Google Scholar

Brysbaert, M., & New, B. (2009). Moving beyond Kučera and Francis: A critical evaluation of current word frequency norms and the introduction of a new and improved word frequency measure for American English. Behavior Research Methods, 41, 977990.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Brysbaert, M., Warriner, A. B., & Kuperman, V. (2014). Concreteness ratings for 40 thousand generally known English word lemmas. Behavior Research Methods, 46, 904911.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Bulté, B., & Housen, A. (2012). Defining and operationalising L2 complexity. In Housen, A., Kuiken, F., & Vedder, I. (Eds.), Dimensions of L2 performance and proficiency: Complexity; accuracy and fluency in SLA. (Vol. 32, pp. 21–46). John Benjamins Publishing Company.Google Scholar

Cedrus Corporation. (2011). Superlab (4.5) [Computer software]. Cedrus Corporation.Google Scholar

Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82, 407428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Cremer, M., Dingshoff, D., de Beer, M., & Schoonen, R. (2010). Do word associations assess word knowledge? A comparison of L1 and L2, child and adult word associations. International Journal of Bilingualism, 15, 187204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Crossley, S. A., Kyle, K., & Salsbury, T. (2016). A usage-based investigation of L2 lexical acquisition: The role of input and output. The Modern Language Journal, 100, 702715.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Daller, H., Milton, J., & Treffers-Daller, J. (2007). Editors’ introduction: Conventions, terminology and an overview of the book. In Daller, H., Milton, J., & Treffers-Daller, J. (Eds.), Modelling and assessing vocabulary knowledge. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Davies, M. (2009). The 385+ million word Corpus of Contemporary American English (1990–2008+): Design, architecture, and linguistic insights. International Journal of Corpus Linguistics, 14, 159190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

de Jong, N. H., Steinel, M. P., Florijn, A. F., Schoonen, R., & Hulstijn, J. H. (2012). Facets of speaking proficiency. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 34, 534.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Dijkstra, T., & van Heuven, W. J. B. B. (2002). The architecture of the bilingual word recognition system: From identification to decision. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 5, 175197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Dóczi, B., & Kormos, J. (2016). Longitudinal developments in vocabulary knowledge and lexical organization. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar

Duyck, W., Desmet, T., Verbeke, L. P. C. C., & Brysbaert, M. (2004). WordGen: A tool for word selection and nonword generation in Dutch, English, German, and French. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36, 488499.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Eguchi, M., & Kyle, K. (2020). Continuing to explore the multidimensional nature of lexical sophistication: The case of oral proficiency interviews. The Modern Language Journal, 104, 381400.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Elgort, I. (2011). Deliberate learning and vocabulary acquisition in a second language: Deliberate learning and vocabulary acquisition in an L2. Language Learning, 61, 367413.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Fitzpatrick, T. (2006). Habits and rabbits: Word associations and the L2 lexicon. EUROSLA Yearbook, 6, 121145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Fitzpatrick, T. (2007). Word association patterns: Unpacking the assumptions. International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 17, 319331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Fitzpatrick, T. (2009). Word association profiles in a first and second language: Puzzles and problems. In T. Fitzpatrick & A. Barfield (Eds.), Lexical processing in second language learners (pp. 38–52). Multilingual Matters.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Fitzpatrick, T. (2012). Word associations. In Chapelle, C. A. (Ed.), The encyclopedia of applied linguistics. Blackwell Publishing Ltd.Google Scholar

Fitzpatrick, T., & Izura, C. (2011). Word association in L1 and L2: An exploratory study of response types, response times, and interlingual mediation. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 33, 373398.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Fitzpatrick, T., Playfoot, D., Wray, A., & Wright, M. J. (2015). Establishing the reliability of word association data for investigating individual and group differences. Applied Linguistics, 36, 2350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Fitzpatrick, T., & Thwaites, P. (2020). Word association research and the L2 lexicon. Language Teaching, 53, 237274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

González-Fernández, B., & Schmitt, N. (2019). Word knowledge: Exploring the relationships and order of acquisition of vocabulary knowledge components. Applied Linguistics. https://doi.org/10.1093/applin/amy057.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Henriksen, B. (1999). Three dimensions vocabulary development. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 21, 303317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Henriksen, B. (2008). Declarative lexical knowledge. In Albrechtsen, D., Haastrup, K., & Henriksen, B. (Eds.), Vocabulary and writing in a first and second language: Processes and development (pp. 2226). Palgrave Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Hill, J., & Morgan, L. (1997). LTP dictionary of selected collocations. LTP.Google Scholar

Hoffman, P., Lambon Ralph, M. A., & Rogers, T. T. (2013). Semantic diversity: A measure of semantic ambiguity based on variability in the contextual usage of words. Behavior Research Methods, 45, 718730.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

JACET (2016). The new JACET list of 8000 basic words. Kirihara Shoten.Google Scholar

Kormos, J. (2006). Speech production and second language acquisition. Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar

Kuperman, V., Stadthagen-Gonzalez, H., & Brysbaert, M. (2012). Age-of-acquisition ratings for 30,000 English words. Behavior Research Methods, 44, 978990.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Kyle, K., Crossley, S. A., & Berger, C. (2017). The tool for the automatic analysis of lexical sophistication (TAALES): Version 2.0. Behavior Research Methods, 50, 10301046.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Laufer, B., & Nation, P. (1999). A vocabulary-size test of controlled productive ability. Language Testing, 16, 3351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Levelt, W. J. M., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 175.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Lucas, M. (2000). Semantic priming without association: A meta-analytic review. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 7, 618630.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Mayor, M. (Ed.) (2013). Longman collocations dictionary and thesaurus. Pearson Education.Google Scholar

McCarthy, P. M., & Jarvis, S. (2010). MTLD, vocd-D, and HD-D: A validation study of sophisticated approaches to lexical diversity assessment. Behavior Research Methods, 42, 381392.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Meara, P. (1982). Word associations in a foreign language: A report on the Birkbeck Vocabulary Project. Nottingham Linguistic Circular, 11, 2838.Google Scholar

Meara, P. (2005). Designing vocabulary tests for English, Spanish and other languages. In Butler, S. C., Gómez-González, M. de los Á., & Doval-Suárez, S. M. (Eds.), The dynamics of language use, (pp. 271286): John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Meara, P., & Wolter, B. (2004). V_Links: Beyond vocabulary depth. Angles on the English Speaking World, 4, 8596.Google Scholar

Mollin, S. (2009). Combining corpus linguistic and psychological data on word co-occurrences: Corpus collocates versus word associations. Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory, 5, 175200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Namei, S. (2004). Bilingual lexical development: A Persian–Swedish word association study. International Journal of Applied Linguistics (United Kingdom), 14(3), 363388.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Nelson, D. L., McEvoy, C. L., & Schreiber, T. A. (2004). The University of South Florida free association, rhyme, and word fragment norms. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36, 402407.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Nissen, H. B., & Henriksen, B. (2006). Word class influence on word association test results. International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 16, 389407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Papageorgiou, S., Tannenbaum, R. J., Bridgeman, B., & Cho, Y. (2015). The association between TOEFL iBT test scores and the Common European Framework of Reference (CEFR) levels (Research Memorandum No. RM-15–06). Educational Testing Service.Google Scholar

Playfoot, D., Balint, T., Pandya, V., Parkes, A., Peters, M., & Richards, S. (2016). Are word association responses really the first words that come to mind? Applied Linguistics, 418, 119.Google Scholar

R Development Core Team. (2014). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. http://www.r-project.org/Google Scholar

Schmitt, N. (2014). Size and depth of vocabulary knowledge: What the research shows. Language Learning, 64, 913951.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Schmitt, N., Schmitt, D., & Clapham, C. (2001). Developing and exploring the behaviour of two new versions of the vocabulary levels test. Language Testing, 18, 5558.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Söderman, T. (1993). Word associations of foreign language learners and native speakers—The phenomenon of a shift in response type and its relevance for lexical development. (Ph.D. thesis). University College of Swansea.Google Scholar

Suzuki, S. (2018). Second language learner corpus: Task-based spontaneous speech by Japanese learners of English. (Unpublished corpus of second language speech).Google Scholar

Wilks, C., & Meara, P. (2002). Untangling word webs: Graph theory and the notion of density in second language word association networks. Second Language Research, 18, 303324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Wolter, B. (2001). Comparing the L1 and L2 mental lexicon: A depth of individual word knowledge model. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 23, 4169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Wolter, B. (2002). Assessing proficiency through word associations: Is there still hope? System, 30, 315329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Wolter, B., & Gyllstad, H. (2011). Collocational links in the L2 mental lexicon and the influence of l1 intralexical knowledge. Applied Linguistics, 32, 430449.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Zareva, A. (2007). Structure of the second language mental lexicon: How does it compare to native speakers’ lexical organization? Second Language Research, 23, 123153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Zareva, A., & Wolter, B. (2012). The “promise” of three methods of word association analysis to L2 lexical research. Second Language Research, 28, 4167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Zenker, F., & Kyle, K. (2021). Investigating minimum text lengths for lexical diversity indices. Assessing Writing, 47, 100505.CrossRefGoogle Scholar